How Traditional Dyslexia Treatments Fail

Digg This
Reddit This
Stumble Now!
Buzz This
Share on Facebook
Bookmark this on Delicious
Share on LinkedIn
Bookmark this on Technorati
Post on Twitter
Google Buzz (aka. Google Reader)

Dyslexia in children and adults is often addressed according to two models, each of which generally focus on linguistic or language-related skills. “These methods emphasize strategy and cognitive development and are not based on a brain processing relationship, which is dysfunctional in dyslexia. Consequently, these techniques have not produced consistent reading improvement.” (Goldstein, 2001) Goldstein’s assessments still ring true today. Even so, many dyslexia treatment options tout an emphasis on “teaching” those with dyslexia words in a clearer way as if other reading educational efforts have somehow failed. The problem is, of course, that dyslexia is not necessarily a letter recognition problem, it is instead a cognitive processing problem. This means that dyslexia treatments based on language skills alone often fail.

One of the most common misconceptions about reading skills and acquisition of reading ability is that reading involves only a simple recognition of letters and subsequent knowledge of how to phonologically string those letters together into words. According to this simplistic model of reading, someone with dyslexia is simply not “seeing” the letters correctly, thus there is a perceived deficit in vision or sight. Although visual cues often play a role in the formation of dyslexia treatments, sight alone is only the tip of a very large iceberg. After all, when we see a sequence of letters, it has no meaning as an object until our brain, working as an integrated network of sensory systems, assigns significance to the abstract grouping of letters. Visual processing disorders, which are NOT related to the ability to see clearly, involve difficulties understanding visual information such as movement, spatial relationships, form, or direction. Such visual processing challenges, together with Central Auditory Processing problems, are frequently found in combination and result in a formal dyslexia diagnosis or poor academic performance.

However, the process is far more complicated on a cognitive level-mere recognition of words and sounds is only the first part in a long series of events that occur quickly and unconsciously in those without dyslexia but this process is “sidetracked” as the two hemispheres of the brain react differently than they would in non-dyslexic readers. Therefore, one of the fundamental flaws of traditional dyslexia treatment is that there is a heavy focus on teaching the words themselves while overlooking the fact that the problem lies in brain’s processing of letters as opposed to some kind of simple lack of understanding of letters, words and phonology.

A great deal of contemporary research focuses on the issue of brain processing in dyslexia treatment with multiple studies examining the delay or miscommunication between the left and right hemisphere of the brain, or problems with specific areas of the brain, including “planum temporal symmetry or angular gyrus dysfunction, that result in reading impairments and do not suggest developmental hemispheric changes as a rationale for dyslexia” (Goldstein 2001). While the results of these imaging-based studies continue to change our view of the cognitive and brain processing end of dyslexia treatment, one thing is clear—simply focusing on “teaching” those with dyslexia the letters or word sounds in a more focused way is simply inadequate. If the basic brain processes that govern the abstract meaning behind words and letters are not improved, then all of the phonics and letter training in the world will likely not solve the challenges that dyslexic readers face.

Source

Goldstein, B., & Obrzut, J. (2001). Neuropsychological Treatment of Dyslexia in the Classroom Setting. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(3), 276.

  • That many dyslexia interventions are based on the concept that dyslexic have difficulties in learning phonics or have difficulty seeing text while ignoring many of the processing difficulties associated with dyslexia is true. But then again almost any generalized statement about dyslexia is likely to be only appropriate for some dyslexics.

    What bothers me as much anything is that there are techniques which address almost all the individual problems common to dyslexia and could be put together for the individual dyslexic but for some reason no one seems to want to offer that particular path. Instead, programs are developed to address common problems of dyslexia and sold as being the answer for all dyslexics.

    My niche is visual dyslexia which is having problems seeing the text. Visual dyslexia is only a problem for a minority of dyslexics. Luckily most visual dyslexics can describe their specific problems in seeing text and being able to describe their visual problems as a criteria can be self diagnosed as visual dyslexics.

    Most of the MRI studies that have investigated vision and dyslexia end up grouping difficulties seeing text with difficulties processing the visual aspects of text. They are two different things and difficult to separate by MRI studies and it is hard to explain the difference in an easily understandable way.

    The easiest way to understand the difference between difficulties seeing the text and processing of visual aspects of text is to first determine if the dyslexics sees text in a clear, uniform, and stable way. Seeing text in motion or in an incomplete manner are the most common visual dyslexia problems. These are visual and not processing problems and can be removed with optical filters so that the text does not move and is complete.

    More information about visual dyslexia can be found at dyslexiaglasses.com .
blog comments powered by Disqus